Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Social Psychology Sexism

Eye-tracking study shows people fixate longer on female aggressors than male ones

by Eric W. Dolan
June 23, 2025
in Sexism
Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Stay on top of the latest psychology findings: Subscribe now!

A new study published in Violence and Gender has found that when people witness aggressive behavior, they pay more attention to female aggressors than to male ones — both in terms of how quickly they look at them and how long they continue to watch. Yet when it comes to judging the intentions and blameworthiness of those actions, participants tended to view male and female aggressors similarly.

These findings, drawn from a series of three experiments, suggest that observers may find female aggression more attention-grabbing because it conflicts with social expectations, not because they see it as more harmful or intentional.

The research was conducted to explore how gender stereotypes shape our attention and judgments in response to aggression. Previous studies have shown that men are more likely than women to use direct physical violence, and that women tend to engage in less visible forms of aggression, such as gossip or social exclusion. As a result, people may have different expectations of aggressive behavior depending on the gender of the person involved.

The researchers wanted to know whether this mismatch between expectation and behavior might cause people to scrutinize female aggressors more closely — and whether that scrutiny would affect how intentional or blameworthy their actions are perceived to be.

To explore these questions, researchers conducted three separate studies. In the first two, participants from the general public were shown images of aggressive interactions between two people, while their eye movements were tracked. Some scenes were ambiguous, meaning it wasn’t entirely clear whether harm was intentional, while others depicted clearly hostile behavior. The third study involved incarcerated violent offenders — individuals who are more familiar with aggressive behavior — to see if they responded differently.

In the first study, 122 adults (mostly in their twenties) viewed ambiguous scenes that could be interpreted in more than one way. These scenes showed two individuals, one potentially causing harm to the other. Each scene had both a male and a female version, so participants would see, for example, a woman shoving another person in one image, and a man doing the same thing in another. While participants looked at the scenes, an eye-tracking device recorded how long they spent looking at each face, and how quickly they first looked at the harm-doer.

After viewing each scene, participants were asked to rate how intentional the harmful behavior seemed and how much blame the aggressor deserved. The researchers found that participants looked faster and longer at the faces of female aggressors compared to male ones. However, they did not see female harm-doers as more intentional than male ones. If anything, women were slightly less likely to be blamed.

In the second study, the researchers replicated this design with another 120 participants, this time including both ambiguous and clearly hostile scenes where the harm-doer’s intentions were unmistakable. Again, the results showed that participants oriented more quickly to female harm-doers and spent more time looking at their faces. But when it came to judgments of intention and blame, there were no clear differences between male and female aggressors. This was true even when the aggression was clearly deliberate.

The third study brought in a very different group: 60 imprisoned individuals who had committed violent offenses, including assault and robbery. This group had more firsthand experience with aggressive behavior and might be expected to have different responses. The researchers again found that participants oriented faster to female aggressors, especially in clearly hostile scenes. In those cases, female faces held their attention longer than male ones.

However, these inmates did not generally judge female aggressors to be more blameworthy than male ones. The only exception came from the female offenders, who were more likely to judge female aggressors as acting intentionally — perhaps reflecting their own lived experiences or expectations.

Taken together, the three studies suggest that people pay more visual attention to female aggressors, even when they don’t judge their actions more harshly. The researchers believe this may be because female aggression stands out as unexpected. Social norms typically associate physical aggression with men, and seeing a woman act violently may prompt people to look longer and more quickly, perhaps to try to make sense of what they’re seeing. This tendency may reflect a kind of cognitive dissonance, where observers try to resolve a mismatch between a stereotype (women are nurturing and nonviolent) and what they are witnessing.

Interestingly, this heightened visual attention did not translate into stronger moral judgment. Across all three studies, female harm-doers were judged as roughly equally intentional as male ones, and sometimes even less blameworthy. This suggests that while female aggression might capture people’s attention, it does not necessarily provoke harsher condemnation — at least in the kinds of situations presented in the study.

There are several possible explanations for this pattern. One is that people may assume that if a woman is being physically aggressive, she must have a good reason for it — possibly because such behavior runs against societal expectations. Another is that people may still hold on to subtle forms of bias that excuse or minimize female aggression more readily than male aggression. This idea, often referred to as “benevolent sexism” or “benevolent genderism,” suggests that women may benefit from assumptions that they are inherently less violent or more morally upright.

The study also highlights the difference between automatic attention and conscious judgment. People’s eyes were drawn more quickly and for longer to female aggressors — an automatic, bottom-up process that likely reflects a need to process surprising or novel stimuli. But when asked to reflect on what they saw and assign blame or intent, participants relied on more deliberate, top-down reasoning, which appeared to be less influenced by the aggressor’s gender.

Future research could explore whether similar patterns hold in real-life scenarios or dynamic video footage, rather than still images. It could also examine whether these visual and cognitive responses are affected by the race, age, or other characteristics of the aggressors. Understanding how these social cues influence perception and judgment has important implications — especially in fields like criminal justice, where assumptions about intent and blame can have serious consequences.

The study, “Judgment and Attention Toward Male and Female Harm-Doers: An Eye-Tracking Investigation in Community Adults and Inmates,” was authored by Anna Zajenkowska, Marta Bodecka, Ewa Duda, Izabela Kazmierczak, Adrianna Jakubowska, Anna Zarazinska-Chrominska, Claire Lawrence, and Łukasz Okruszek.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

New research reveals aging shifts gender stereotypes in unexpected ways
Sexism

New research reveals aging shifts gender stereotypes in unexpected ways

May 19, 2025

A new meta-analysis challenges the idea that older women face “double jeopardy” due to age and gender. While younger and middle-aged women are viewed more positively than men, perceptions of older women and men appear to converge in later life.

Read moreDetails
Women underestimate their spatial intelligence—even when they perform just as well as men
Cognitive Science

Women underestimate their spatial intelligence—even when they perform just as well as men

May 6, 2025

New research shows women underestimate their spatial intelligence, even when they perform just as well as men. This gender gap in self-perception—shaped by personality traits like narcissism and modesty—could help explain why fewer women pursue STEM careers.

Read moreDetails
Changing implicit stereotypes helps men see themselves as more caring
Sexism

Changing implicit stereotypes helps men see themselves as more caring

April 17, 2025

Men may identify more with caring and connection when stereotypes linking these traits to women are disrupted, according to a new study.

Read moreDetails
Neuroticism linked to lower brain volume in individuals unable to adapt themselves to situations in life
Relationships and Sexual Health

New study sheds light on why objectification lowers women’s relationship satisfaction

March 20, 2025

A study finds that women who feel objectified by their partners experience lower personal power, which in turn is linked to reduced relationship satisfaction.

Read moreDetails
Even feminist men can struggle with female proposals, study suggests
Relationships and Sexual Health

Even feminist men can struggle with female proposals, study suggests

March 14, 2025

Marriage norms are shifting, but proposals remain traditional. New research explores women who buck the trend and propose, revealing their experiences and the social reactions they face.

Read moreDetails
Feeling objectified by partner linked to fewer orgasms and more emotional labor for women
Relationships and Sexual Health

Feeling objectified by partner linked to fewer orgasms and more emotional labor for women

March 13, 2025

A new study finds that when women feel objectified by their partner, they report fewer orgasms and greater emotional labor in sex.

Read moreDetails
Men in childcare are seen as less warm, moral, and competent than women
Sexism

Men in childcare are seen as less warm, moral, and competent than women

March 13, 2025

Why are men so underrepresented in childcare? A new study in Sex Roles explores the stereotypes shaping public perceptions of male childcare workers—and how these biases influence support for greater gender diversity in the field.

Read moreDetails
Men who reject gender equality are more likely to commit intimate partner violence
Sexism

Men who reject gender equality are more likely to commit intimate partner violence

March 8, 2025

Men with more traditional, gender-inequitable beliefs were over twice as likely to report committing intimate partner violence, according to a new study.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Online camming can help men feel more comfortable in their own skin, study suggests

Spider fear inflates size perception, highlighting the role of emotion in threat assessment

Brain connectivity shift across puberty may explain autism risk in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome

How people end romantic relationships: New study pinpoints three common break up strategies

Mild cognitive impairment linked to lower mindfulness and weaker brain connections for emotion regulation

Anxious minds don’t always fixate on danger, according to new study

More time in bed benefits morning and evening-type teens differently, new study suggests

Cognitive inflexibility amplifies risk of disordered exercise in men

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy